The potential roles of multilingualism and language structural distance in neurocognitive adaptation to language experience

Authors

  • Vincent DeLuca UiT The Arctic University of Norway

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1558/jmtp.25486

Keywords:

bilingualism, cognitive outcomes, individual differences, multilingualism, neuroplasticity

Abstract

A growing body of research shows that individual differences in bilingual language experience variably contribute to neurocognitive adaptations. The trends in this work are beginning to show a specificity of adaptations to the nature and degree of bilingual experiences – for example, duration of use, language switching, context and intensity of engagement. However, several other factors likely further influence these bilingualism-induced neural and cognitive adaptations. Two such factors may play a modulatory role but are currently under-researched in this field. The first is how such adaptations are affected by the addition of a third or more language and the second is the role of language structural distance in shaping neurocognitive adaptation trajectories. Regarding the role of multilingualism, previous research suggests differences in neurocognitive adaptation to multiple languages over two, although results are mixed. It is possible that, like bilingualism, individual differences in engagement with a third (or more) language might also variably condition neurocognitive adaptation, but this remains an unanswered empirical question. Regarding the role of language structural distance, there are plausible reasons to predict that the relative distance in language pairings would condition the nature or trajectory of bilingualism-induced adaptations, but this is also an understudied topic. I review the existing literature covering these factors, discuss the potential mechanisms of adaptation to each and propose some potential ways of incorporating these factors into future research.

Author Biography

  • Vincent DeLuca, UiT The Arctic University of Norway

    Vincent DeLuca is an associate professor in the neurocognition of bilingualism at UiT The Arctic University of Norway. His research focuses primarily on how different aspects of bilingual language experience variably impact brain structure, function and several cognitive processes.

References

Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernández, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F. and Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr287

Abutalebi, J. and Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(4), 689–698. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000225

Abutalebi, J., Rosa, P. A. Della, Castro Gonzaga, A. K., Keim, R., Costa, A. and Perani, D. (2013). The role of the left putamen in multilingual language production. Brain and Language, 125(3), 307–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.03.009

Anderson, J. A. E., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Bellana, B., Luk, G. and Bialystok, E. (2018). Language and cognitive control networks in bilinguals and monolinguals. Neuropsychologia, 117, 352–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.023

Antoniou, K., Grohmann, K. K., Kambanaros, M. and Katsos, N. (2016). The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition, 149, 18–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.002

Antoniou, M. and Wright, S. M. (2017). Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02217

Bak, T. H. (2016). Cooking pasta in La Paz. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 699–717. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16002.bak

Barbu, C. A., Gillet, S. and Poncelet, M. (2020). Investigating the effects of language-switching frequency on attentional and executive functioning in proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01078

Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E. and Dussias, P. E. (2021). Phasic pupillary responses reveal differential engagement of attentional control in bilingual spoken language processing. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 23474. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-03008-1

Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E. and Kroll, J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(6), 1022–1047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770

Becker, M., Schubert, T., Strobach, T., Gallinat, J. and Kühn, S. (2016). Simultaneous interpreters vs. professional multilingual controls: Group differences in cognitive control as well as brain structure and function. NeuroImage, 134, 250–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.079

Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C. L., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A. and Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24(1), 40–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M. and Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001

Bice, K. and Kroll, J. F. (2019). English only? Monolinguals in linguistically diverse contexts have an edge in language learning. Brain and Language, 196, 104644. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104644

Bice, K., Yamasaki, B. L. and Prat, C. S. (2020). Bilingual language experience shapes resting-state brain rhythms. Neurobiology of Language, 1(3), 288–318. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00014

Blasi, D. E., Henrich, J., Adamou, E., Kemmerer, D. and Majid, A. (2022). Over-reliance on English hinders cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(12), 1153–1170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.09.015

Burgaleta, M., Sanjuán, A., Ventura-Campos, N., Sebastián-Gallés, N. and Ávila, C. (2016). Bilingualism at the core of the brain: Structural differences between bilinguals and monolinguals revealed by subcortical shape analysis. NeuroImage, 125, 437–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.073

Calabria, M., Costa, A., Green, D. W. and Abutalebi, J. (2018). Neural basis of bilingual language control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 221–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13879

Calvo, N. and Bialystok, E. (2021). Electrophysiological signatures of attentional control in bilingual processing: Evidence from proactive interference. Brain and Language, 222, 105027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105027

Carter, F., DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A. and Krott, A. (2023). Functional neural architecture of cognitive control mediates the relationship between individual differences in bilingual experience and behaviour. NeuroImage, 273, 120085. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120085

Champoux-Larsson, M.-F., & Dylman, A. S. (2021). Different measurements of bilingualism and their effect on performance on a Simon task. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42(2), 505–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716420000661

Chiswick, B. R. and Miller, P. W. (2005). Linguistic distance: A quantitative measure of the distance between English and other languages. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 26(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790710508668395

Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Sorge, G. B. and Bialystok, E. (2020). Continuous effects of bilingualism and attention on Flanker task performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 1106–1111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000036

Coderre, E. L., Smith, J. F., Van Heuven, W. J. B. and Horwitz, B. (2016). The functional overlap of executive control and language processing in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 471–488. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000188

Coderre, E. L. and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2014). The effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01070

Costa, A., Hernández, M. and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/jcognition.2006.12.013

Dash, T., Berroir, P., Joanette, Y. and Ansaldo, A. I. (2019). Alerting, orienting, and executive control: The effect of bilingualism and age on the subcomponents of attention. Frontiers in Neurology, 10(OCT), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.01122

de Bruin, A. (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033

DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E. and Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(15), 7565–7574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1811513116

DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A. and Krott, A. (2020). Understanding bilingual brain function and structure changes? U bet! A unified bilingual experience trajectory model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100930

Dijkstra, T. and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728902003012

Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J. and Homer, B. D. (2015). Examining the bilingual advantage on conflict resolution tasks: A meta-analysis. In Proceedings of 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 596–601.

Feleke, T. L., Gooskens, C. and Rabanus, S. (2020). Mapping the dimensions of linguistic distance: A study on South Ethiosemitic languages. Lingua, 243, 102893. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102893

Festman, J. (2021). Learning and processing multiple languages: The more the easier? Language Learning, 71, 121–162. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12437

Fyndanis, V., Cameron, S., Hansen, P. B., Norvik, M. I. and Simonsen, H. G. (2023). Multilingualism and verbal short-term/working memory: Evidence from academics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(3), 490–503. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000621

Gallo, F., Myachykov, A., Nelyubina, M., Shtyrov, Y., Kubiak, J., Terekhina, L., & Abutalebi, J. (2023b). Linguistic distance dynamically modulates the effects of bilingualism on executive performance in aging. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000743

Gallo, F., Terekhina, L., Shtyrov, Y. and Myachykov, A. (2023). Neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve effects in the caudate nucleus of young bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000457

García-Pentón, L., Fernández García, Y., Costello, B., Duñabeitia, J. A. and Carreiras, M. (2016). The neuroanatomy of bilingualism: how to turn a hazy view into the full picture. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(3), 303–327. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1068944

Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728998000133

Green, D. W. and Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377

Grogan, A., Green, D. W., Ali, N., Crinion, J. T. and Price, C. J. (2009). Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2690–2698. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp023

Grundy, J. G. (2020). The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: An updated quantitative analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 4(2), 177–199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00062-5

Grundy, J. G., Anderson, J. A. E. and Bialystok, E. (2017). Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396(1), 183–201. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13333

Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., Baum, S., Klein, D. and Titone, D. (2018). Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia, 117, 123–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.037

Gullifer, J. W., Pivneva, I., Whitford, V., Sheikh, N. A. and Titone, D. (2023). Bilingual language experience and its effect on conflict adaptation in reactive inhibitory control tasks. Psychological Science, 34(2), 238–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221113764

Gullifer, J. W. and Titone, D. (2020). Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 283–294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000026

Gullifer, J. W. and Titone, D. (2021). Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(3), 414–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000933

Hämäläinen, S., Sairanen, V., Leminen, A. and Lehtonen, M. (2017). Bilingualism modulates the white matter structure of language-related pathways. NeuroImage, 152, 249–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.081

Hartanto, A. and Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016

Hartanto, A. and Yang, H. (2020). The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 609–633. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000672

Hervais-Adelman, A. and Babcock, L. (2020). The neurobiology of simultaneous interpreting: Where extreme language control and cognitive control intersect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(4), 740–751. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000324

Hervais-Adelman, A., Egorova, N. and Golestani, N. (2018). Beyond bilingualism: Multilingual experience correlates with caudate volume. Brain Structure and Function, 223(7), 3495–3502. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-018-1695-0

Hervais-Adelman, A., Moser-Mercer, B., Michel, C. M. and Golestani, N. (2015). FMRI of simultaneous interpretation reveals the neural basis of extreme language control. Cerebral Cortex, 25(12), 4727–4739. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu158

Hofweber, J., Marinis, T. and Treffers-Daller, J. (2020). How different code-switching types modulate bilinguals’ executive functions: A dual control mode perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(4), 909–925. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000804

Jafari, Z., Villeneuve, C., Thompson, J. and Koravand, A. (2023). Subcortical plasticity and enhanced neural synchrony in multilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(2), 307–316. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000645

Jouravlev, O., Mineroff, Z., Blank, I. A. and Fedorenko, E. (2021). The small and efficient language network of polyglots and hyper-polyglots. Cerebral Cortex, 31(1), 62–76. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa205

Kaiser, A. C., Eppenberger, L. S., Smieskova, R., Borgwardt, S., Kuenzli, E., Radue, E. W., Nitsch, C. and Bendfeldt, K. (2015). Age of second language acquisition in multilinguals has an impact on gray matter volume in language-associated brain areas. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00638

Kalamala, P., Chuderski, A., Szewczyk, J., Senderecka, M. and Wodniecka, Z. (2023). Bilingualism caught in a net: A new approach to understanding the complexity of bilingual experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 157–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001263

Kalamala, P., Walther, J., Zhang, H., Diaz, M. T., Senderecka, M. and Wodniecka, Z. (2022). The use of a second language enhances the neural efficiency of inhibitory control: An ERP study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(1), 163–180. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000389

Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C. and Kempe, V. (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.929580

Kirk, N. W., Kempe, V., Scott-Brown, K. C., Philipp, A. and Declerck, M. (2018). Can monolinguals be like bilinguals? Evidence from dialect switching. Cognition, 170, 164–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.001

Korenar, M., Treffers-Daller, J. and Pliatsikas, C. (2023). Dynamic effects of bilingualism on brain structure map onto general principles of experience-based neuroplasticity. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 3428. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-30326-3

Kousaie, S. and Phillips, N. A. (2017). A behavioural and electrophysiological investigation of the effect of bilingualism on aging and cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 94, 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.013

Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A. and Kroff, J. R. V. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 56, 229–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394393-4.00007-8

Kuhl, P. K., Stevenson, J., Corrigan, N. M., van den Bosch, J. J. F., Can, D. D. and Richards, T. L. (2016). Neuroimaging of the bilingual brain: Structural brain correlates of listening and speaking in a second language. Brain and Language, 162, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.07.004

Kutlu, E. and Hayes-Harb, R. (2023). Towards a just and equitable applied psycholinguistics. Applied Psycholinguistics, 44(3), 293–300. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716423000280

Lee, Y. Y. (2022). A conceptual analysis of typological distance and its potential consequences on the bilingual brain. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(9), 3333–3346. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2022.2052790

Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A. and Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142

Leivada, E., Westergaard, M., Duñabeitia, J. A. and Rothman, J. (2021). On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(1), 197–210. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000358

Li, P., Legault, J. and Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001

Li, P., Zhang, F., Yu, A. and Zhao, X. (2020). Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 938–944. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918001153

Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W. and Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 651–662. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891100054X

Luk, G. and Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574

Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M. and Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 16808–16813. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011

Luk, G., Mesite, L. and Leon Guerrero, S. (2020). Onset age of second language acquisition and fractional anisotropy variation in multilingual young adults. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100937

Lundquist, B. and Vangsnes, Ø. A. (2018). Language separation in bidialectal speakers: Evidence from eye tracking. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01394

Luo, D., Kwok, V. P. Y., Liu, Q., Li, W., Yang, Y., Zhou, K., Xu, M., Gao, J.-H. and Tan, L. H. (2019). Microstructural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Brain and Language, 196, 104654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104654

Madrazo, A. R. and Bernardo, A. B. I. (2018). Measuring two types of inhibitory control in bilinguals and trilinguals: Is there a trilingual advantage? Psychological Studies, 63(1), 52–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-018-0439-9

Marin-Marin, L., Costumero, V., Ávila, C. and Pliatsikas, C. (2022). Dynamic effects of immersive bilingualism on cortical and subcortical grey matter volumes. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886222

Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and Price, C. J. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431(7010), 757. https://doi.org/10.1038/431757a

Melinger, A. (2021). Do elevators compete with lifts? Selecting dialect alternatives. Cognition, 206, 104471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104471

Mohades, S. G., Van Schuerbeek, P., Rosseel, Y., Van De Craen, P., Luypaert, R. and Baeken, C. (2015). White-matter development is different in bilingual and monolingual children: a longitudinal DTI study. PLoS One, 10(2), e0117968. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117968

Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gómez-Ariza, C. J. and Bajo, M. T. (2015). Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.014

Morrison, C. and Taler, V. (2023). ERP differences between monolinguals and bilinguals: The role of linguistic distance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(2), 293–306. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000657

Navarro, E., DeLuca, V. and Rossi, E. (2022). It takes a village: Using network science to identify the effect of individual differences in bilingual experience for theory of mind. Brain Sciences, 12(4), 487. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040487

Nichols, E. S. and Joanisse, M. F. (2016). Functional activity and white matter microstructure reveal the independent effects of age of acquisition and proficiency on second-language learning. NeuroImage, 143, 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.053

Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mikulinsky, R., Masuda, S. and Mason, L. (2019). On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.12.001

Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A. and Sawi, O. M. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014

Pereira Soares, S. M., Kubota, M., Rossi, E. and Rothman, J. (2021). Determinants of bilingualism predict dynamic changes in resting state EEG oscillations. Brain and Language, 223, 105030. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105030

Pereira Soares, S. M., Prystauka, Y., DeLuca, V. and Rothman, J. (2022). Type of bilingualism conditions individual differences in the oscillatory dynamics of inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.910910

Perovic, A., Filipovic Durdevic, D. and Halupka-Rešetar, S. (2023). The effect of bilingualism on executive functions when languages are similar: A comparison between Hungarian–Serbian and Slovak–Serbian young adult bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 51(3), 561–581. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-022-01345-8

Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Multilingualism and brain plasticity. In J. W. Schwieter (ed.), The handbook of the neuroscience of multilingualism (pp. 230–251). Wiley.

Pliatsikas, C. (2020). Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 459–471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000130

Pliatsikas, C., DeLuca, V., Moschopoulou, E. and Saddy, J. D. (2017). Immersive bilingualism reshapes the core of the brain. Brain Structure and Function, 222(4), 1785–1795. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-016-1307-9

Pliatsikas, C. and Luk, G. (2016). Executive control in bilinguals: A concise review on fMRI studies. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(4), 699–705. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000249

Pliatsikas, C., Moschopoulou, E. and Saddy, J. D. (2015). The effects of bilingualism on the white matter structure of the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(5), 1334–1337. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414183112

Poarch, G. J. and van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013

Poarch, G. J., Vanhove, J. and Berthele, R. (2019). The effect of bidialectalism on executive function. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 612–628. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006918763132

Putnam, M. T., Carlson, M. and Reitter, D. (2018). Integrated, not isolated: Defining typological proximity in an integrated multilingual architecture. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02212

Radman, N., Jost, L., Dorood, S., Mancini, C. and Annoni, J.-M. (2021). Language distance modulates cognitive control in bilinguals. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 24131. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-02973-x

Richardson, F. M., Thomas, M. S. C., Filippi, R., Harth, H. and Price, C. J. (2010). Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across the lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 943–954. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21238

Ross, J. and Melinger, A. (2017). Bilingual advantage, bidialectal advantage or neither? Comparing performance across three tests of executive function in middle childhood. Developmental Science, 20(4), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12405

Rossi, E., Cheng, H., Kroll, J. F., Diaz, M. T. and Newman, S. D. (2017). Changes in white-matter connectivity in late second language learners: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02040

Rothman, J. (2015). Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 179–190. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891300059X

Rothman, J., Alemán Bañón, J. and González Alonso, J. (2015). Neurolinguistic measures of typological effects in multilingual transfer: Introducing an ERP methodology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01087

Rothman, J., Bayram, F., DeLuca, V., Di Pisa, G., Duñabeitia, J. A., Gharibi, K., Hao, J., Kolb, N., Kubota, M., Kupisch, T., Laméris, T., Luque, A., van Osch, B., Pereira Soares, S. M., Prystauka, Y., Tat, D., Tomic, A., Voits, T. and Wulff, S. (2022). Monolingual comparative normativity in bilingualism research is out of ‘control’: Arguments and alternatives. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716422000315

Rothman, J., González Alonso, J. and Puig-Mayenco, E. (2019). Third language acquisition and linguistic transfer. Cambridge University Press.

Salig, L. K., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Slevc, L. R. and Novick, J. M. (2021). Moving from bilingual traits to states: Understanding cognition and language processing through moment-to-moment variation. Neurobiology of Language, 2(4), 487–512. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00046

Schwieter, J. W. and Ferreira, A. (2013). Language selection, control, and conceptual-lexical development in bilinguals and multilinguals. In J. W. Schwieter (ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (pp. 241–266). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.38.15sch

Singh, N. C., Rajan, A., Malagi, A., Ramanujan, K., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Raghunathan, P., Weekes, B. S. and Abutalebi, J. (2018). Microstructural anatomical differences between bilinguals and monolinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 995–1008. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000438

Sorge, G., Toplak, M. E. and Bialystok, E. (2017). Interactions between levels of attention ability and levels of bilingualism in children’s executive functioning. Developmental Science, 20(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12408

Sörman, D. E., Hansson, P. and Ljungberg, J. K. (2019). Different features of bilingualism in relation to executive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00269

Stasenko, A., Matt, G. E. and Gollan, T. H. (2017). A relative bilingual advantage in switching with preparation: Nuanced explorations of the proposed association between bilingualism and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(11), 1527–1550. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000340

Stocco, A., Yamasaki, B. L., Natalenko, R. and Prat, C. S. (2014). Bilingual brain training: A neurobiological framework of how bilingual experience improves executive function. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18(1), 67–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006912456617

Sulpizio, S., Del Maschio, N., Del Mauro, G., Fedeli, D. and Abutalebi, J. (2020). Bilingualism as a gradient measure modulates functional connectivity of language and control networks. NeuroImage, 205, 116306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116306

Titone, D. A. and Tiv, M. (2023). Rethinking multilingual experience through a systems framework of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921001127

Tiv, M., Gullifer, J. W., Feng, R. Y. and Titone, D. (2020). Using network science to map what Montréal bilinguals talk about across languages and communicative contexts. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100913. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100913

Tiv, M., Kutlu, E., O’Regan, E. and Titone, D. (2022). Bridging people and perspectives: General and language-specific social network structure predict mentalizing across diverse sociolinguistic contexts. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology / Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 76(4), 235–250. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000273

Tran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M. and Yoshida, H. (2019). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 714–732. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000160

Treffers-Daller, J., Ongun, Z., Hofweber, J. and Korenar, M. (2020). Explaining individual differences in executive functions performance in multilinguals: The impact of code-switching and alternating between multicultural identity styles. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.561088

Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S., Gupta, A., Pablos, L. and Schiller, N. O. (2023). When left is right: The role of typological similarity in multilinguals’ inhibitory control performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 165–178. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000426

Wagner, D., Bekas, K. and Bialystok, E. (2023). Does language entropy shape cognitive performance? A tale of two cities. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728923000202

Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R. and Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916648859

Wichmann, S., Holman, E. W., Bakker, D. and Brown, C. H. (2010). Evaluating linguistic distance measures. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications, 389(17), 3632–3639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2010.05.011

Xie, W., Altarriba, J. and Ng, B. C. (2022). Bilingualism, culture, and executive functions: Is there a relationship? Languages, 7(4), 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247

Yamasaki, B. L., Stocco, A. and Prat, C. S. (2018). Relating individual differences in bilingual language experiences to executive attention. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(9), 1128–1151. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1448092

Yang, H., Tng, G. Y. Q., Ng, G. R. and Ng, W. Q. (2023). Bilingual interactional contexts predict executive functions in older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 36–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000190

Yee, J., DeLuca, V. and Pliatsikas, C. (2023). The effects of multilingualism on brain structure, language control and language processing. In J. Cabrelli, A. Chaouch-Orozco, J. González Alonso, S. M. P. Soares, E. Puig-Mayenco and J. Rothman (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Third Language Acquisition (pp. 577–605). Cambridge University Press.

Published

2024-05-24

Issue

Section

Special Issue: Guest Editors Anne Beatty-Martinez & Sarah Surrain

How to Cite

DeLuca, V. (2024). The potential roles of multilingualism and language structural distance in neurocognitive adaptation to language experience. Journal of Multilingual Theories and Practices. https://doi.org/10.1558/jmtp.25486