The influence of age of acquisition, language proficiency and language use on Singapore English consonant perception

Authors

  • Kastoori Kalaivanan DUKE-NUS Medical School
  • Firqin Sumartono National University of Singapore
  • Ying-Ying Tan Nanyang Technological University

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1558/jmbs.19174

Keywords:

Singapore English, language use, proficiency, consonants, age of acquisition (AoA)

Abstract

The present study investigates the extent to which age of acquisition (AoA), English language proficiency and use influence consonant perception in Singapore English (SgE). Consonantal pairs of interest were presented in a gating paradigm to determine the amount of linguistic information participants required for accurate consonant identification as a measure of perceptual acuity. Only the [th]–[?] and [t]–[?] phonetic contrasts emerged sensitive to differences in AoA of English, with the early AoA group outperforming the late AoA group. Language proficiency and use did not significantly influence consonant perception. The findings are discussed in light of how AoA may interact with the universal difficulty and distributional regularities of speech contrasts to affect speech processing. Future studies should investigate a wider range of SgE speakers and identify distributional regularities of speech sounds to gain a more comprehensive picture of the factors that influence consonant perception in SgE.

References

Amengual, M. (2014). The perception and production of language-specific mid-vowel contrasts: shifting the focus to the bilingual individual in early language input conditions. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(2), 133–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006914544988

Archila-Suerte, P., Zevin, J. & Hernandez, A. E. (2015). The effect of age of acquisition, socioeducational status, and proficiency on the neural processing of second language speech sounds. Brain and Language, 141, 35–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.005

Archila-Suerte, P., Zevin, J., Bunta, F. & Hernandez, A. E. (2012). Age of acquisition and proficiency in a second language independently influence the perception of non-native speech. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England), 15(1), 190–201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000125

Bao, Z. M. (2003). Social stigma and grammatical autonomy in nonnative varieties of English. Language in Society, 32, 23–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404503321025

Bernhardt, B. M., Ignatova, D., Amoako, W., Aspinall, N., Marinova-Todd, S., Stemberger, J. P. & Yokota, K. (2019). Bulgarian consonant acquisition in preschoolers with typical versus protracted phonological development. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 1(2), 143–81. https://doi.org/10.1558/jmbs.v1i2.11801

Birdsong, D., Gertken, L. M. & Amengual, M. (2012). Bilingual language profile: an easy-to-use instrument to assess bilingualism. Austin, TX: COERLL, University of Texas at Austin.

Bolton, K. (2008). English in Asia, Asian Englishes, and the issue of proficiency. English Today, 24(2), 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1017/S026607840800014X

Bolton, K., Botha, W. & Bacon-Shone, J. (2017). English-medium instruction in Singapore higher education: policy, realities and challenges. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 38(10), 913–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2017.1304396

Brannen, K. (2002). The role of perception in differential substitution. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 47(1–2), 1–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008413100018004

Callan, D. E., Tajima, K., Callan, A. M., Kubo, R., Masaki, S. & Akahane-Yamada, R. (2003). Learning-induced neural plasticity associated with improved identification performance after training of a difficult second-language phonetic contrast. NeuroImage, 19(1), 113–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00020-X

Cavallaro, F., Ng, B. C. & Tan, Y. Y. (2020). Singapore English. In K. Bolton, W. Botha & A. Kirkpatrick (eds), The Handbook of Asian Englishes, 419–47. Chichester: John Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118791882.ch18

Census of Singapore. 2010. Singapore census of population 2010. Singapore: Department of Statistics, Ministry of Trade and Industry.

Consonni, M., Cafiero, R., Marin, D., Tettamanti, M., Iadanza, A., Fabbro, F. & Perani, D. (2013). Neural convergence for language comprehension and grammatical class production in highly proficient bilinguals is independent of age of acquisition. Cortex, 49(5), 1252–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.009

Curtiss, S. R. (1977). Genie: a linguistic study of a modern day ‘wild child’. New York: Academic Press.

Darcy, I., Daidone, D. & Kojima, C. (2013). Asymmetric lexical access and fuzzy lexical representations in second language learners. The Mental Lexicon, 8(3), 372–420. https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.8.3.06dar

Davis, M. H., Evans, S., McCarthy, K., Evans, L., Giannakopoulou, A. & Taylor, J. S. H. (2019). Lexical learning shapes the development of speech perception until late adolescence. Preprint. Retrieved from https://psyarxiv.com/ktsey. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ktsey

De Carli, F., et al. (2015). Language use affects proficiency in Italian–Spanish bilinguals irrespective of age of second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 324–39. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000054

de Leeuw, E. & Celata, C. (2019). Plasticity of native phonetic and phonological domains in the context of bilingualism. Journal of Phonetics, 75, 88–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2019.05.003

Deterding, D. (2007). Singapore English. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780748630967

Eckman, F. R. (1977). Markedness and the contrastive analysis hypothesis. Language Learning, 27(2), 315–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1977.tb00124.x

Eckman, F. R. (2013). Markedness differential hypothesis (MDH). Language, 15, 335–70.

Flege, J. E. (1987). A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages? Applied Linguistics, 8(2), 162–77. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/8.2.162

Flege, J. E. & Bohn, O. S. (2021). The revised speech learning model (SLM-r). Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress (pp. 3–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108886901.002

Flege, J. E., Aoyama, K. & Bohn, O. S. (2021). The revised speech learning model (SLM-r) applied. In W. Wayland (ed.), Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress (pp. 84–118). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108886901.003

Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. & Meador, D. (1999). Native Italian speakers’ perception and production of English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(5), 2973–87. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.428116

Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J. & MacKay, I. R. (1996). Factors affecting the production of word-initial consonants in a second language. Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Variation, 10, 47–73. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.10.03fle

Flege, J. E., Takagi, N. & Mann, V. (1996). Lexical familiarity and English-language experience affect Japanese adults’ perception of /r/ and /l/. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99(2), 1161–73. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.414884

Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H. & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(1), 78–104. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2638

Friedmann, N. & Rusou, D. (2015). Critical period for first language: the crucial role of language input during the first year of life. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 35, 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.003

Garcia-Sierra, A., Ramírez-Esparza, N. & Kuhl, P. K. (2016). Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.10.004

Garcia-Sierra, A., et al. (2011). Bilingual language learning: an ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.07.002

Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E., Davis, B. L. & MacNeilage, P. F. (2000). Contingencies governing the production of fricatives, affricates, and liquids in babbling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(3), 341–63. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400003039

Goldrick, M. (2017). Encoding of distributional regularities independent of markedness: evidence from unimpaired speakers. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(7–8), 476–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1421149

Grosjean, F. (1996). Gating. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11(6), 597–604. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909696386999

Hanington, L. & Renandya, W. A. (2017). Developing English language skills in the Singapore higher education context. In English Education at the Tertiary Level in Asia (pp. 152–67). Abingdon: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315391588-9

Hernandez, A. E. & Li, P. (2007). Age of acquisition: its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 638. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.638

Hernandez, A. E., Bates, E. A. & Avila, L. X. (1996). Processing across the language boundary: a cross-modal priming study of Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(4), 846. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.846

Hopp, H. & Schmid, M. S. (2013). Perceived foreign accent in first language attrition and second language acquisition: the impact of age of acquisition and bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(2), 361–94. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000737

Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., Tigi, M. & Moselle, M. (1994). Reexamining the critical period hypothesis: a case of successful adult SLA in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 73–98. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100012596

Itard, J. M. G. (1962). The wild boy of Aveyron (trans. G. Humphrey & M. Humphrey). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall (Originally published, 1894.).

Iverson, P., Hazan, V. & Bannister, K. (2005). Phonetic training with acoustic cue manipulations: a comparison of methods for teaching English /r/–/l/ to Japanese adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(5), 3267–3278. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2062307

Jekiel, M. (2012). The evolution of English dental fricatives: variation and change. Thesis, University of Adam Mickiewics, Poland.

Johnson, J. S. & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0

Johnson, K. (2012). Acoustic and auditory phonetics, 3rd edition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

Kalaivanan, K., Sumartono, F. & Tan, Y. Y. (2020). The homogenization of ethnic differences in Singapore English? A consonantal production study. Language and Speech, 64(1), 123–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830920925510

Kim, C. (2018). Voiceless interdental fricative in Singapore English. Studies in English Language & Literature, 44(4), 47–65. https://doi.org/10.21559/aellk.2018.44.4.003

Kkese, E. & Petinou, K. (2017). Perception abilities of L1 Cypriot Greek listeners-types of errors involving plosive consonants in L2 English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9417-3

Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11,850–11,857. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850

Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S. & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00468.x

Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice, 2(12), 59–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/21548331.1967.11707799

Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830

Logan, J. S., Lively, S. E. & Pisoni, D. B. (1991). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: a first report. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(2), 874–86. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1894649

MacLeod, A. A., Sutton, A., Trudeau, N. & Thordardottir, E. (2011). The acquisition of consonants in Québécois French: a cross-sectional study of pre-school aged children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13(2), 93–109. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2011.487543

Maye, J., Werker, J. F. & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3

Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H. & Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: a two-country study. Acta Paediatrica, 102(2), 156–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12098

Nelson, C. (2020). The younger, the better? Speech perception development in adolescent vs. adult L3 learners. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistic Meeting, 6(1), 27–58. https://doi.org/10.2478/yplm-2020-0005

Nichols, E. S. & Joanisse, M. F. (2016). Functional activity and white matter microstructure reveal the independent effects of age of acquisition and proficiency on second-language learning. NeuroImage, 143, 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.053

Pallier, C., Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception. Cognition, 64(3), B9–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00030-9

Patkowski, M. S. (1980). The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language Language Learning, 30(2), 449–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1980.tb00328.x

Perani, D., et al. (1996). Brain processing of native and foreign languages. NeuroReport-International Journal for Rapid Communications of Research in Neuroscience, 7(15), 2439–2444. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199611040-00007

Querleu, D., Renard, X., Versyp, F., Paris-Delrue, L. & Crèpin, G. (1988). Fetal hearing. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 28(3), 191–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-2243(88)90030-5

Rahman, A., Yap, N. T. & Darmi, R. (2018). The association between vocabulary size and language dominance of bilingual Malay–English undergraduates. 3L: The Southeast Asian Journal of English Language Studies, 24(4), 85–101. https://doi.org/10.17576/3L-2018-2404-07

Reh, R. K., Hensch, T. K. & Werker, J. F. (2021). Distributional learning of speech sound categories is gated by sensitive periods. Cognition, 213, article 104653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104653

Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Klarman, L., Garcia-Sierra, A. & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Neural patterns to speech and vocabulary growth in American infants. NeuroReport, 16(5), 495–98. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200504040-00015

Saffran, J. R. (2020). Statistical language learning in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 49–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12355

Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Soto-Faraco, S. (1999). Online processing of native and non-native phonemic contrasts in early bilinguals. Cognition, 72(2), 111–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00024-4

Sebastián-Gallés, N., Echeverría, S. & Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 240–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.001

Streeter, L. A. (1976). Language perception of 2-month-old infants shows effects of both innate mechanisms and experience. Nature, 259(5538), 39–41. https://doi.org/10.1038/259039a0

Sulpizio, S., Del Maschio, N., Del Mauro, G., Fedeli, D. & Abutalebi, J. (2020). Bilingualism as a gradient measure modulates functional connectivity of language and control networks. NeuroImage, 205, 116306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116306

Tan, Y. Y. (2012). To r or not to r: social correlates of /r/ in Singapore English. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2012(218), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl-2012-0057

Tan, Y. Y. (2014). English as a ‘mother tongue’ in Singapore. World Englishes, 33(3), 319–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/weng.12093

Vicente, M., Calandruccio, L., Miller, M. K., Browning, J. M., Oleson, J. & Leibold, L. J. (2019). Language proficiency and dominance considerations when working with Spanish–English bilingual adults. American Journal of Audiology, 28(3), 724–29. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJA-19-0028

Werker, J. F. & Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 173–96. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104

Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3

Published

2022-06-22

Issue

Section

Articles

How to Cite

Kalaivanan, Kastoori, Firqin Sumartono, and Ying-Ying Tan. 2022. “The Influence of Age of Acquisition, Language Proficiency and Language Use on Singapore English Consonant Perception”. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech 4 (1): 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1558/jmbs.19174.