Collaborative, responsive behavior in peer interaction
Children with and without Down syndrome and hearing impairment
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.24422Keywords:
children, Down syndrome, hearing impairment, peer interaction, alignmentAbstract
Background: Interactive work with children who have unequal cognitive resources and unequal access to language can be challenging. This article explores the collaborative labor involved in establishing alignment between a child with Down syndrome in combination with hearing impairment (DS-HI) and a hearing peer without DS, during a shared interactive book reading activity in an inclusive educational setting.
Method: Video recording of children in a naturally occurring, shared book reading activity is analyzed using conversation analysis. The recording took place in the children’s school.
Results: The situated shared book reading is regulated by interaction rules. A pattern of collaborative, responsive behavior where the children align to each other’s initiatives is identified within these interaction rules.
Discussion and conclusion: The shared book reading format has a scaffolding function in the interaction. The findings illustrate how a typically developing peer is able to align to the sensory and cognitive capacities displayed in the child with DS-HI, and how this facilitates co-created meaning-making in the interaction.
References
Antaki, C., and Widdicombe, S. (Eds.) (1998). Identities in talk. London: SAGE.
Austeng, M. E., Akre, H., Falkenberg, E.-S., Øverland, B., Abdelnoor, M., and Kværner, K. J. (2013). Hearing level in children with Down syndrome at the age of eight. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(7), 2251–2256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.006
Baker, C. (1977). Regulators and turn-taking in American Sign Language. In L. A. Friedman (Ed.), On the other hand: New perspectives on American Sign Language (pp. 215–236). New York: Academic Press.
Beechey, T., Buchholz, J. M., and Keidser, G. (2020). Hearing impairment increases communication effort during conversations in noise. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 63(1), 305–320. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00201
Bolden, G. B., Mandelbaum, J., and Wilkinson, S. (2012). Pursuing a response by repairing an indexical reference. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(2), 137–155. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2012.673380
Bourke, J., Ricciardo, B., Bebbington, A., Aiberti, K., Jacoby, P., Dyke, P., …, and Leonard, H. (2008). Physical and mental health in mothers of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Pediatrics, 153(3), 320–326.e323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.02.047
Broth, M., and Keevallik, L. (Eds.) (2020). Multimodal interaktionsanalys. Lund: Studentlitteratur.
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Campbell, C., Landry, O., Russo, N., Flores, H., Jacques, S., and Burack, J. A. (2013). Cognitive flexibility among individuals with Down syndrome: assessing the influence of verbal and nonverbal abilities. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 118(3), 193–200. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-118.3.193
Carpenter, B. (1995). Tell me about Katie – Attitudes of mainstream 7–8 year olds to a peer with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 3(2), 45–52. https://doi.org/10.3104/case-studies.50
Channell, M. M., Mattie, L. J., Hamilton, D. R., Capone, G. T., Mahone, E. M., Sherman, S. L., …, and Kalb, L. G. (2021). Capturing cognitive and behavioral variability among individuals with Down syndrome: A latent profile analysis. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 13(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-021-09365-2
Clarke, M., and Wilkinson, R. (2008). Interaction between children with cerebral palsy and their peers 2: Understanding initiated VOCA-mediated turns. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 24(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434610701390400
Clarke, M., and Wilkinson, R. (2010). Communication aid use in children’s conversation: Time, timing and speaker transfer. In H. Gardner and M. Forrester (Eds.), Analysing interactions in childhood (pp. 249–266). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Clarke, M., and Wilkinson, R. (2013). Communicative competence in children’s peer interaction. In N. Norén, C. Samuelsson, and C. Plejert (Eds.), Aided communication in everyday interaction (pp. 23–57). Guildford: J & R Press.
Conway, C. M., Pisoni, D. B., and Kronenberger, W. G. (2009). The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 275–279. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01651.x
De Vos, C., Torreira, F., and Levinson, S. C. (2015). Turn-timing in signed conversations: Coordinating stroke-to-stroke turn boundaries. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 183–195. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00268
Dolva, A. S., Hemmingsson, H., Gustavsson, A., and Borell, L. (2010). Children with Down syndrome in mainstream schools: Peer interaction in activities. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 25(3), 283–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2010.492941
Drew, P. (2013). Turn design. In J. Sidnell and T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 131–149). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118325001.ch7
Eilertsen, L.-J. (2014). Maintaining intersubjectivity when communication is challenging: Hearing impairment and complex needs. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 47(4), 353–379. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2014.958278
Eilertsen, L.-J. (2016a). Cochlear implants for deaf children with complex needs (eBulletin). http://raisingandeducatingdeafchildren.org/node/272
Eilertsen, L.-J. (2016b). Deltakelse i barnefellesskap mellom barn med og uten sammensatte vansker, der hørselsnedsettelse inngår. Doktoravhandling, Universitetet i Oslo, Oslo.
Eilertsen, L.-J. (2017). Langtidsutbytte av cochleaimplantat hos barn med sammensatte vansker. Dansk audiologopædi, 53(1), 16–20.
Erting, C. J., Prezioso, C., and O’Grady Hynes, M. (1990). The interactional context of deaf mother-infant communication. In V. Volterra and C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 97–106). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-74859-2_9
Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Martin, C. L., Moss, A., and Reesing, A. (2012). The effects of young children’s affiliations with prosocial peers on subsequent emotionality in peer interactions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 569–585. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02073.x
Farkas, L., Cless, J. D., Cless, A. W., Nelson Goff, B. S., Bodine, E., and Edelman, A. (2019). The ups and downs of Down syndrome: A qualitative study of positive and negative parenting experiences. Journal of Family Issues, 40(4), 518–539. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18812192
Forrester, M. A. (2015). Early social interaction: A case comparison of developmental pragmatics and psychoanalytic theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107045217
Girard-Groeber, S. (2015). The management of turn transition in signed interaction through the lens of overlaps. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 196–214. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00741
Goodwin, C. (2017). Co-operative action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781139016735
Grove, N., and Launonen, K. (Eds.) (2019). Manual sign acquisition in children with developmental disabilities. New York: Nova.
Gunnarsdottir, B., and Bateman, A. (2017). Toddler agency and conversation analysis. Early Childhood Folio, 21(1), 33–38. https://doi.org/10.18296/ecf.0030
Hallberg, L. R. M., and Carlsson, S. G. (1991). A qualitative study of strategies for managing a hearing impairment. British Journal of Audiology, 25(3), 201–211. https://doi.org/10.3109/03005369109079853
Hepburn, A., and Bolden, G. B. (2013). The conversation analytic approach to transcription. In J. Sidnell and T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 57–76). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118325001.ch4
Houston, D. M. (2022). A framework for understanding the relation between spoken language input and outcomes for children with cochlear implants. Child Development Perspectives, 16(1), 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12443
Ionescu, T. (2012). Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New Ideas in Psychology, 30(2), 190–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.11.001
Levinson, S. C., and Torreira, F. (2015). Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 10–26. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731
Mondada, L. (2019). Conventions for multimodal transcription. https://www.lorenzamondada.net/multimodal-transcription
Nicastri, M., Giallini, I., Amicucci, M., Mariani, L., De Vincentiis, M., Greco, A., .…, and Mancini, P. (2021). Variables influencing executive functioning in preschool hearing-impaired children implanted within 24 months of age: an observational cohort study. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology, 278(8), 2733–2743. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00405-020-06343-7
Nightengale, E., Yoon, P., Wolter-Warmerdam, K., Daniels, D., and Hickey, F. (2017). Understanding hearing and hearing loss in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Audiology, 26(3), 301–308. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_AJA-17-0010
Norén, N., Samuelsson, C., and Plejert, C. (2013). Dialogical perspectives on aided communication. In N. Norén, C. Samuelsson, and C. Plejert (Eds.), Aided communication in everyday interaction (pp. 1–22). Guildford: J & R Press.
Pang, J., Beach, E. F., Gilliver, M., and Yeend, I. (2019). Adults who report difficulty hearing speech in noise: An exploration of experiences, impacts and coping strategies. International Journal of Audiology, 58(12), 851–860. https://doi.org/10.1080/14992027.2019.1670363
Rasmussen, G. (2014). Inclined to better understanding – the coordination of talk and ‘leaning forward’ in doing repair. Journal of Pragmatics, 65, 30–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2013.10.001
Sacco, S., Bouis, C., Gallard, J., Pichot, A., Blondiaux, E., Marey, I., …, and Mircher, C. (2022). Psychomotor development in infants and young children with Down syndrome – a prospective, repeated measure, post-hoc analysis. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 188(3), 818–827. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.62587
Schegloff, E. A. (1987). Analyzing single episodes of interaction: An exercise in conversation analysis. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50(2), 101–114. https://doi.org/10.2307/2786745
Schegloff, E. A. (1997). Practices and actions: Boundary cases of other-initiated repair. Discourse Processes, 23(3), 499–545. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539709545001
Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., and Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53(2), 361–382. https://doi.org/10.2307/413107
Skedsmo, K. (2020). Multiple other-initiations of repair in Norwegian Sign Language. Open Linguistics, 6(1), 532–566. https://doi.org/10.1515/opli-2020-0030
Steensig, J. (2020). Conversation analysis and affiliation and alignment. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.), The concise encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 248–252). Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell.
Steensig, J., and Drew, P. (2008). Introduction: Questioning and affiliation/disaffiliation in interaction. Discourse Studies, 10(1), 5–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445607085581
Stivers, T. (2008). Stance, alignment, and affiliation during storytelling: When nodding Is a token of affiliation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 41(1), 31–57. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810701691123
Stivers, T. (2013). Sequence organisation. In J. Sidnell and T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 191–209). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118325001.ch10
Stivers, T., Mondada, L., and Steensig, J. (2011). Knowledge, morality and affiliation in social interaction. In The morality of knowledge in conversation (pp. 3–24). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511921674.002
Stivers, T., Sidnell, J., and Bergen, C. (2018). Children’s responses to questions in peer interaction: A window into the ontogenesis of interactional competence. Journal of Pragmatics, 124, 14–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2017.11.013
Swanwick, R., and Watson, L. (2007). Parents sharing books with young deaf children in spoken English and in BSL: The common and diverse features of different language settings. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(3), 385–405. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm004
Syrjämäki, M., Pihlaja, P., and Sajaniemi, N. K. (2019). Enhancing peer interaction in early childhood special education: Chains of children’s initiatives, adults’ responses and their consequences in play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 47(5), 559–570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-019-00952-6
Van Dijk, R., Nelson, C., Postma, A., and van Dijk, J. (2010). Deaf children with severe multiple disabilities: Etiologies, intervention, and assessment. In M. Marschark and P. E. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (vol. 2, pp. 172–191). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Von Tetzchner, S. (2018). Child and adolescent psychology. London: Taylor & Francis. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315742113
Vonen, A. M., and Peterson, P. R. (2019). Sign language legislation in Norway. In M. De Meulder, J. J. Murray, and R. L. McKee (Eds.), The legal recognition of sign languages: Advocacy and outcomes around the world (pp. 191–205): Bristol: Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781788924016-014
Voutilainen, L., Henttonen, P., Stevanovic, M., Kahri, M., and Peräkylä, A. (2019). Nods, vocal continuers, and the perception of empathy in storytelling. Discourse Processes, 56(4), 310–330. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2018.1498670
Wilkinson, R. (2019). Atypical interaction: Conversation analysis and communicative impairments. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 52(3), 281–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2019.1631045