Reading trajectories in children with language disorders

Capturing variability of change over time

Authors

  • Ryan Nelson University of Louisiana at Lafayette
  • Jack S. Damico University of Colorado at Boulder
  • Holly L. Damico University of Louisiana at Lafayette
  • Karen Lynch University of Colorado at Boulder
  • Kathleen J. Abendroth University of Louisiana at Lafayette
  • Christine Weill University of Louisiana at Lafayette
  • Laura E. Arrington University of Louisiana at Lafayette
  • Amanda Percle University of Louisiana at Lafayette

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.19134

Keywords:

reading development, developmental trajectories, child language disorders, qualitative research

Abstract

Background: The literature on typically developing children’s literacy acquisition provides a conventional model of development; however, little is available regarding how children with communicative disorders develop literacy abilities over time. This study describes the temporal trajectories of reading development in three children with diagnoses of language disorders and histories of reading difficulty.

Method: A qualitative research design employing ethnographic data collection and analysis procedures was used. One male (age 8 years 9 months) and two females (ages 7 years 8 months and 7 years 11 months) identified as having impaired language and a history of reading difficulty were followed for 10 months as they engaged in literacy instruction.

Results: Each child demonstrated a unique trajectory of literacy improvement. Trajectories of development were observed across the complexity of the materials read, as well as across variables reflective of comprehension. Variables included formal miscue analysis data, retelling of stories read, and text complexity.

Discussion/conclusion: This study suggests that each language-impaired individual should be considered separately in how they progress and in how progression is manifested. If norm-referenced, standardized evaluation methods had been applied, or if only one sampling period had been used, the changes and progress noted in each of these participants may have been missed.

Author Biographies

  • Ryan Nelson, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Ryan L. Nelson is an associate professor and the department head of the Department of Communicative Disorders at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. His research interests include language and literacy development in childhood with language disorders and interactional approaches to treating this population.

  • Jack S. Damico, University of Colorado at Boulder

    Jack Damico is a professor of speech, language, and hearing sciences at the University of Colorado at Boulder. He held the position of Doris B. Hawthorne Eminent Scholar in Special Education and Communication for 28 years at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. His research interests involve qualitative research methodologies in communicative sciences and disorders in adults, children, and literacy.

  • Holly L. Damico, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Holly Damico is an associate professor in the Department of Communicative Disorders and the co-director of the language and literacy project at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. Her areas of clinical interest and research involve language and literacy acquisition and disorders, qualitative research methods, and apprenticeship models of learning.

  • Karen Lynch, University of Colorado at Boulder

    Karen Lynch is an adjunct assistant professor at the University of Colorado at Boulder. She has worked as an SLP in public schools, early intervention, and rehabilitation hospitals, and has performed swallow studies for a mobile imaging company. She was an assistant professor at Southeastern University in Hammond, Louisiana, and an adjunct professor and clinical supervisor at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. She served as president, president-elect, and communications director of the Louisiana Speech Language and Hearing Association.

  • Kathleen J. Abendroth, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Kathleen J Abendroth is a clinical supervisor at University of Louisiana at Lafayette. She has worked as an assistant professor at Southeastern Louisiana University, as an SLP in public schools and private practice. She has published in the areas of autism, literacy, language impairments, and qualitative research.

  • Christine Weill, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Christine Weill is the clinical director of the Department of Communicative Disorders at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. Her clinical and research interests include autism spectrum disorders, language and literacy, literacy development, and social interaction.

  • Laura E. Arrington, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Laura Arrington is a visiting assistant professor in the Department of Communicative Disorders at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. She has worked as a speech-language pathologist for over 15 years in a variety of settings. Her primary areas of interest are strengths-based intervention, literacy, mild traumatic brain injury, and qualitative research methodology.

  • Amanda Percle, University of Louisiana at Lafayette

    Amanda Percle is a speech-language pathologist in the Department of Communicative Disorders at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette. She is currently pursuing a doctoral degree in applied speech and language sciences. Her primary interests include literacy, language disorders in children, eye-movement analysis, and qualitative research methodology.

References

Agar, M. (2013). The lively science. Remodeling human social research. Minneapolis: Mill City Press.

Benjamin, R. G. (2012). Reconstructing readability: Recent developments and recommendations in the analysis of text difficulty. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 63–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9181-8

Bridwell, N. (1992). Clifford’s kitten. New York: Scholastic.

Carle, E. (1984). The very busy spider. New York: Philomel Books.

Catts H. W., Fey M. E., Tomblin J. B., and Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/093)

Clay, M. (1998). By different paths to common outcomes. York: Stenhouse Publishers.

Creswell, J. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

Cunningham, J. W., Spadorcia, S. A., Erickson, K., Koppenhaver, D. A., Sturm, J. M., and Yoder, D. E. (2005). Investigating the instructional supportiveness of leveled texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 410–427. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.40.4.2

Damico, H. L., and Nelson, R. L. (2017). Encounters in literacy: Clinical implications based on qualitative research. Journal of Interactional Research in Communicative Disorders, 8, 162–182. https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.32303

Damico, H. L., Damico, J. S., Nelson, R. L., Weill, C., and Maxwell, J. (2017). Infusing meaning and joy back into books: Reclaiming literacy in the treatment of young children with autism spectrum disorder. In R. J. Meyer and K. F. Whitmore (Eds.), Reclaiming early childhood literacies: Narratives of hope, power, and vision (pp. 109–119). New York: Routledge.

Damico, J. S. (2014). Aiming for explication: Reflections on the qualitative research process. In M. J. Ball, N. Muller, and R. Nelson (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research in communication disorders (pp. 343–356). London: Psychology Press.

Damico, J. S., and Tetnowski, J. A. (2014). Triangulation. In H. Copes and C. Forsyth (Eds.), Encyclopedia of social deviance (pp. 751–754). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Delton, J. (1988). Two good friends. New York: Knolf Books.

Eisner, E. W. (1991). The enlightened eye: Qualitative inquiry and the enhancement of educational practice. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.

Fennacy, J. (1998). Becoming readers and writers over time. In C. Weaver (Ed.), Practicing what we know: Informed reading instruction (pp. 462–478). Urbana: National.

Flurkey, A. D. (2008). Reading flow. In A. Flurkey, E. Paulson, and K. Goodman (Eds.), Scientific realism in studies of reading (pp. 267–304). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003064329-30

Fountas, I., and Pinnell, G. S. (1999). Matching books to readers. Using leveled books to guided reading, K-3. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Fountas, I., and Pinnell, G. S. (2017). The Fountas & Pinnell literacy continuum, expanded edition: A tool for assessment, planning, and teaching. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Gillam, S. L., and Gillam, R. B. (2006). Making evidence-based decisions about child language intervention in schools. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 304–315. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2006/035)

Goodman, K. (1996). On reading: A common-sense look at the nature of language and the science of reading. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Goodman, Y. M., and Goodman, K. S. (1994). To err is human: Learning about language processes by analyzing miscues. In R. B. Ruddell, M. R. Ruddell, and H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading. Newark: International Reading Association.

Goodman, Y., Watson, D., and Burke, C. (2005). Reading miscue inventory (2nd ed.). Katonah: Richard C. Owen Publishers, Inc.

Hatcher, P. (2000). Predictors of reading recovery book levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 67–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.00103

Hiebert, E. H., and Pearson, P. D. (2010). An examination of current text difficulty indices with early reading texts. Reading Research Report No. 10-01. Santa Cruz: TextProject, Inc. Council of Teachers of English.

Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction–integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163

Lansky, B. (1991). Kids pick the funniest poems. New York: Meadowbrook.

Leppänen, U., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., and Nurmi, J. (2004). Development of reading skills among preschool and primary school pupils. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(1), 72–93. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.39.1.5

Mesmer, H. A. (2008). Tools for matching readers to texts. New York: Guilford Press.

Moran, A. (2003). Boots for Beth. Orlando: Harcourt Inc.

Nelson, R. (2004). Investigation of the process of improved literacy construction in individuals with poor reading abilities and an identification of language impairment. Dissertation Abstracts International, 65(11), 5674B. (UMI No. 3153731).

Nelson, R., and Damico, J. S. (2006). Qualitative research in literacy acquisition: A framework for investigating reading in children with language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 631–639. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200500266760

Nelson, R. L., Abendroth, K., and Lynch, K. (2014). Ethnography. In M. J. Ball, N. Muller, and R. Nelson (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research in communication disorders. (pp. 39–54). London: Psychology Press.

Park, B. (1998). Junie B. Jones smells something fishy. Boston: Random House.

Parrila, R. K., Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Nurmi, J.-E., and Kirby, J. (2005). Development of individual differences in reading: Results from longitudinal studies in English and Finnish. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 299–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.299

Peng, P., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Elleman, A. M., Kearns, D. M., Gilbert, J. K., Compton, D. L., Cho, E., and Patton, S. (2018). A longitudinal analysis of the trajectories and predictors of word reading and reading comprehension development among at-risk readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 52(3), 195–208. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418809080

Percle, A., Arrington, L., Flurkey, A. D., Damico, H., Weill, C., Damico, J., and Nelson, R. L. (2020). Illuminating the complexity of oral reading fluency: A multiple lens approach. Literacy Research: Theory, Method, and Practice, 69(1), 358–376. https://doi.org/10.1177/2381336920937269

Prutting, C. (1979). Process: The action of moving forward progressively from one point to another on the way to completion. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 44, 3–30. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4401.03

Rasinski, T. (2004). Assessing reading fluency. Honolulu: Pacific Resources for Education and Learning. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED483166.pdf

Sadler, M. (1983) It’s not easy being a bunny. New York: Random House.

Skibbe, L., Grimm, K., Stanton-Chapman, T., Justice, L., Pence, K., and Bowles, R. (2008). Reading trajectories of children with language difficulties from preschool through fifth grade. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 475–486. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0016)

Small, D. (1988). Imogene’s antlers. New York: Dragonfly Books.

Stevenson, J. (2000). The most amazing dinosaur. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.

Sullivan, A. L., Kohli, N., Farnsworth, E. M., Sadeh, S., and Jones, L. (2017). Longitudinal models of reading achievement of students with learning disabilities and without disabilities. School Psychology Quarterly, 32(3), 336–349. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000170

Sulzby, E. (1985). Children’s emergent reading of favorite storybooks: A developmental study. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 458–481. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.20.4.4

Thompson, L. (2003). Little quack. New York: Simon & Schuster Children’s Publishing.

Weaver, C. (1998). Reconsidering a balanced approach to reading. Urbana: National Council of Teachers of English.

Published

2021-11-20

Issue

Section

Articles

How to Cite

Nelson, R. ., Damico, J. S. ., Damico, H. L. ., Lynch, K. ., Abendroth, K. J. ., Weill, C. ., Arrington, L. E. ., & Percle, A. . (2021). Reading trajectories in children with language disorders: Capturing variability of change over time. Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders, 11(2), 171–193. https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.19134