Action bids in children with speech impairments
The case of marking
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1558/rcsi.18065Keywords:
marking, conversation analysis, Down syndrome, assessments, action formation, repetitionAbstract
The paper illustrates a practice, which we have called ‘marking’, observed in play interactions between parents and children with Down syndrome (DS) aged 3–8 years. Markings are minimal turns that rely on prosody, embodied resources and indexicality to foreground events within an ongoing activity and convey a stance toward them. Markings can be both retrospective and prospective (i.e. referring to a just-occurred or an incipient event). As first pair parts, they are open action bids that prompt recipients to display their co-orientation towards the referent. Responses from parents (i.e. second markings) can take the form of repeats or expansions; after prospective marking the recipient can also add support to the incipient activity the child has marked. We discuss marking as the core constituent of a larger family of actions for ‘sharing noteworthiness’, but also as a designedly undetermined action bid with specific conversational uses for children and adults alike.
References
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1977). From gesture to the first word: On cognitive and social prerequisites. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (eds), Interaction, Conversation, and the Development of Language (pp. 247–307). New York: Wiley.
Burdelski, M. & Morita, E. (2017) Young children’s initial assessments in Japanese. In A. Bateman & A. Church (Eds.), Children’s Knowledge-in-Interaction: Studies in Conversation Analysis (pp. 231–255) Singapore: Springer.
Clibbens, J. (2001). Signing and lexical development in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7(3), 101–105.
Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind and Language, 25(2), 141–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01384.x
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
Enfield, N. J. & Sidnell, J. (2017). On the concept of action in the study of interaction. Discourse Studies, 19(5), 515–535. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445617730235
Fidler, D. J. (2005). The emerging Down syndrome behavioral phenotype in early childhood: Implications for practice. Infants & Young Children, 18(2), 86–103.
Gardner, H. (2009). Applying conversation analysis to interactions with atypically developing children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 23(8), 551–554. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200802491173
Gardner, H., & Forrester, M. (Eds.). (2009). Analysing interactions in childhood: Insights from conversation analysis. London: Wiley.
Goffman, E. (1979). Footing. Semiotica, 25, 1–29.
Goodwin, C. (2007). Interactive footing. In E. Holt & R. Clift (eds), Reporting Talk: Reported Speech in Interaction (pp. 16–46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Goodwin, M., Cekaite, A. & Goodwin, C. (2012). Emotion as stance. In M.-L. Sorjonen & A. Perakyla (eds), Emotion in Interaction (pp. 16–41). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Heritage J. (1984) A change of state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (eds), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis, (pp. 299–345) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heritage, J. (2011). Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: Empathic moments in interaction. In T. Stivers, L. Mondada & J. Steensig (eds), The Morality of Knowledge in Conversation (pp. 159–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heritage, J. (2019) The design of polar questions: Two preferences and their management. Keynote lecture, IIEMCA conference, Mannheim, Germany, 2–5 July.
Jefferson, G. (1987), On exposed and embedded correction in conversation. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (eds), Talk and Social Organisation, (pp. 86–100) Clevedon: Multilingual Matters
Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. In G. H. Lerner (ed.), Conversation Analysis: Studies from the First Generation (pp. 13–31). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Keel, S. (2015). Young children’s embodied pursuits of a response to their initial assessments. Journal of Pragmatics, 75, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2014.10.005
Kendrick, K. H. & Drew, P. (2016). Recruitment: Offers, requests, and the organization of assistance in interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 49(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2016.1126436
Kidwell, M. & Zimmerman, D. H. (2007). Joint attention as action. Journal of Pragmatics, 39(3), 592–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2006.07.012
Laws, G. & Bishop, V. M. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39(1), 45–64.
Leavens, D. A., Sansone, J., Burfield, A., Lightfoot, S., O’Hara, S. & Todd, B. K. (2014). Putting the ‘Joy’ in joint attention: affective-gestural synchrony by parents who point for their babies. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 879. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00879
Legerstee, M., Bowman, T. G. & Fels, S. (1992). People and objects affect the quality of vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1002/edp.2430010304
Liszkowski, U. (2006). Infant pointing at twelve months: Communicative goals, motives, and social-cognitive abilities. In N. Enfield & Levinson, S. (eds), The Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Interaction (pp. 153–178). Oxford: Berg.
Martin, G. E., Klusek, J., Estigarribia, B. & Roberts, J. E. (2009). Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Topics in Language Disorders, 29(2), 112–132. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181a71fe1
Mondada, L., (2007). Transcript variations and the indexicality of transcribing practices. Discourse Studies, 9(6), 809–821. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445607082581
Morita, E. (2005). Negotiation of Contingent Talk: The Japanese Interactional Particles Ne and Sa. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Newton, R. (2015) Neuropsychiatry of Down syndrome. In R. W. Newton, S. Puri & L. Marder (eds), Down Syndrome: Current Perspectives, (pp. 238–269). London: Mac Keith Press.
Ninio, A., Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A. & Rollins, P. R. (1994). Classifying communicative acts in children’s interactions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 27(2), 157–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(94)90039-6
Nomikou, I. & Rohlfing, K. J. (2011). Language does something: body action and language in maternal input to three-month-olds. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3(2), 113–128. https://doi.org/10.1109/tamd.2011.2140113
Nye, J., Fluck, M. & Buckley, S. (2001). Counting and cardinal understanding in children with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7(2), 68–78.
Ogden, R. (2006). Phonetics and social action in agreements and disagreements. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1752–1775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2005.04.011
Pursi, A., Lipponen, L. & Sajaniemi, N. K. (2018). Emotional and playful stance taking in joint play between adults and very young children. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 18, 28–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lcsi.2018.03.002
Rondal, J. A. (1987). Down’s syndrome. In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (eds), Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances (pp. 165-176). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
Rossi, G. (2018). Composite social actions: The case of factual declaratives in everyday interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 51(4), 379–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2018.1524562
Searle, J. (1969). Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sigman, M. et al. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), 1–139. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-5834.00002
Singer Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W. & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 345–370. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540683
Stivers, T. & Rossano, F. (2010). Mobilizing response. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 43(1), 3–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810903471258
Strid, E. & Cekaite, A. (2018). Affective stances and announcements in young children’s social interactions: Everyday morality in recruiting a conversational partner. Presented at the 5th International Conference on Conversation Analysis (ICCA), Loughborough, UK, 13 July.
Svennevig, J. (2004). Other-repetition as display of hearing, understanding and emotional stance. Discourse Studies, 6(4), 489–516.
Wells, B. & Corrin, J. (2004). Prosodic resources, turn-taking and overlap in children’s talk-in-interaction. In E. Couper-Kuhlen & C. Ford (eds), Sound Patterns in Interaction, (pp. 119–144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Wells, G. (1974). Learning to code experience through language. Journal of Child Language, 1(2), 243–269.
Wright, D. E. & Bray I. (2000). Estimating birth prevalence of Down’s syndrome. Journal of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, 5, 2 89–97.